M1 recruitment during interleaved practice is important for encoding, not just consolidation, of skill memory

M1 recruitment during interleaved practice is important for encoding, not just consolidation, of skill memory

Methods

Participants

Participants assigned to IP with cathodal stimulation at the right M1, and two sham conditions, each including a separate group of 18 participants, received training in either an RP or IP format. Real (IP-ctDCS) or sham (RP-Sham, IP-Sham) stimulation was applied during the entire 20-min period of practice. Participants were blinded to the stimulation condition. All participants completed an informed written consent approved by Texas A&M University’s Institutional Review Board before any involvement in the experiment.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)

We targeted the right M1 with a 1 (times) 1 tDCS electrode montage. Real stimulation consisted of a 2 mA current applied via a 25 cm2 (5 (times) 5 cm) cathode and a 35 cm2 (5 (times) 7 cm) reference electrode covered by saline-soaked sponges, resulting in a maximum current density of 0.08 mA/cm2 administered using a 9 V battery-driven stimulator (tDCS Stimulator; TCT Research Limited, Hong Kong). The cathode was located at the right M1 (i.e., C4, International 10–20 system) and was paired with a reference electrode above the left supraorbital region.

This placement system has established accuracy for targeting M1, with the current flow simulation shown in Fig. 3. The current flow associated with this electrode montage was modeled using HD-ExploreTM (Soterix Medical Inc., New York, NY) and revealed heightened current flow at regions described as right M1 in the human motor area template33. Participants in sham stimulation conditions (RP-Sham, IP-Sham) involved the same electrode configuration, but stimulation was only delivered for 30-s at the beginning and end of the training period.

Fig. 3: tDCS montage used during practice in IP.
M1 recruitment during interleaved practice is important for encoding, not just consolidation, of skill memory

a Cathodal stimulation for the interleaved practice, real tDCS condition involved placement of the cathode 20% of the auricular measurement from Cz (determined based on the International 10–20 system) which placed this electrode above C4 with a reference electrode at the left supraorbital region. Participants in sham stimulation conditions (Repetitive-Sham, Interleaved-Sham) involved the same electrode configuration, but stimulation was only delivered for 30-s at the beginning and end of the training period37. b, c The expected field intensity of 2 mA tDCS at C4 and the associated current flow for this electrode montage was modeled using HD-ExploreTM (Soterix Medical Inc., New York, NY). This figure illustrates the right M1, noted with a white circle.

Full size image

Motor sequence learning task

We employed a motor sequence learning task, specifically, a discrete sequence production task20. To ensure that we studied cortical activity related to motor execution rather than stimulus-response mappings and probabilities34,35, we did not use the four-finger (one per button) design of the traditional serial reaction time task paradigm7. Instead, our participant used only their left index finger for all keys17,18,19, which creates real, non-isometric motor demands in the context of a key pressing task36 (Fig. 1a). A standard keyboard was used. Individuals executed a keypress to a visual signal that was spatially compatible with the position of the key. Once a correct key was pressed, the next visual signal in the sequence was presented. The primary dependent variable assessing motor sequence performance was total response time (TT), which was the interval from the presentation of the first stimulus to the correct execution of the final keypress of the motor sequence.

All individuals completed nine blocks of 21 trials each, totaling 189 trials of practice. These 189 trials were divided between three 6-key motor sequences (63 trials per sequence). The content of each block depended on practice schedule (Fig. 1b). For RP, each block contained 21 repetitions of a single sequence; for IP, each block contained 7 trials of each of the three sequences, presented in counterbalanced order. Test blocks were presented in RP format without stimulation. All participants completed 3 Test blocks at 4 occasions: prior to training (Pre), immediately after training (Post), 5 min, 6 h later, and 24 h later (Fig. 1b).

Statistical analyses

To ensure the normality of the data used in this study, we assessed it using the Shapiro-Wilk test. The test statistic (W) was 0.97, with a corresponding p value of 0.21. As the p-value was greater than 0.05, we failed to reject the null hypothesis that the data were normally distributed; therefore, we used parametric ANOVA for all analyses. In addition, a Bonferroni adjustment was made when conducting post-hoc multiple comparisons. An initial 3 (Group: RP-Sham, IP-Sham, IP-ctDCS) between-subject ANOVA was conducted to assess if individuals assigned to each of the experimental conditions exhibited similar performance during the baseline test block. The evaluation of online performance (during training) was assessed using two separate two-way ANOVAs with repeated measures on the last factor: (1) Group (IP-ctDCS, IP-Sham) (times) Block and (2) Group (IP-Sham, RP-Sham) (times) Block. To determine the effect of ctDCS in M1 and practice structure in offline performance gains, two separate two-way ANOVAs with repeated measures on the last factor were performed: (1) Group (IP-ctDCS, IP-Sham) (times) Time Point (Post5min, Post6h, Post24h) and (2) Group (IP-Sham, RP-Sham) (times) Time Point (Post5min, Post6h, Post24h). This approach, conducting two separate analyses for (1) IP-Sham vs. RP-Sham and (2) IP-Sham vs. IP-ctDCS allowed for a focused examination of the effects of practice schedule and stimulation on M1 on online/offline learning, enhancing clarity in interpretation. If we find significant effects, we used Tukey’s Honest Significant Differences post-hoc assessment to identify differences in means.

Related Articles

Skill dependencies uncover nested human capital

Modern economies require increasingly diverse and specialized skills, many of which depend on the acquisition of other skills first. Here we analyse US survey data to reveal a nested structure within skill portfolios, where the direction of dependency is inferred from asymmetrical conditional probabilities—occupations require one skill conditional on another. This directional nature suggests that advanced, specific skills and knowledge are often built upon broader, fundamental ones. We examine 70 million job transitions to show that human capital development and career progression follow this structured pathway in which skills more aligned with the nested structure command higher wage premiums, require longer education and are less likely to be automated. These disparities are evident across genders and racial/ethnic groups, explaining long-term wage penalties. Finally, we find that this nested structure has become even more pronounced over the past two decades, indicating increased barriers to upward job mobility.

Transcranial direct current stimulation targeting the bilateral IFG alters cognitive processes during creative ideation

This study investigated whether transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) targeting the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) can alter the thinking process and neural basis of creativity. Participants’ performance on the compound remote associates (CRA) task was analyzed considering the semantic features of each trial after receiving different tDCS protocols (left cathodal and right anodal, L + R−; right cathodal and left anodal, L−R+; and Sham). Moreover, we constructed and compared 80 prediction models of CRA performance for each group based on task-related functional connectivity. Results showed that L + R− stimulation improved performance in semantically bundled CRA trials, while L−R+ stimulation enhanced performance in trials with greater semantic distance. Furthermore, alpha-band task connectivity models for the L + R− group showed inferior performance and greater left frontal lateralization than other two groups. These findings suggest that tDCS targeting the bilateral IFG alters cognitive processes during creative ideation rather than enhancing or impairing an established thinking process.

Neurophysiological insights into catecholamine-dependent tDCS modulation of cognitive control

Goal-directed behavior requires resolving both consciously and subconsciously induced response conflicts. Neuronal gain control, which enhances processing efficacy, is crucial for conflict resolution and can be increased through pharmacological or brain stimulation interventions, though it faces inherent physical limits. This study examined the effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (atDCS) and methylphenidate (MPH) on conflict processing. Healthy adults (n = 105) performed a flanker task, with electroencephalography (EEG) used to assess alpha and theta band activity (ABA, TBA). Results showed that combining atDCS with MPH enhanced cognitive control and reduced response conflicts more effectively than atDCS alone, particularly when both conflict types co-occurred. Both atDCS and atDCS + MPH exhibited similar task-induced ABA and TBA modulations in the (pre)supplementary motor area, indicating heightened gain control. Overlapping neuroanatomical effects in mid-superior frontal areas suggest that atDCS and MPH share a common neuronal mechanism of gain control, especially in high-conflict/-demand situations.

Comparison of stimulation sites enhancing dual-task performance using transcranial direct current stimulation in Parkinson’s disease

This pilot randomized crossover study aimed to compare the effects of stimulating various transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) target sites to improve dual-task performance in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD). Nineteen patients with idiopathic PD completed four sessions of 2 mA anodal tDCS for 20 min at randomly assigned sites: the primary motor cortex (M1), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and sham stimulation. Anodal M1 tDCS induced statistically significant improvements in single-task and cognitive dual-task timed up and go test. Additionally, enhancements were noted in the color-word Stroop test and trail-making test-Trail B following left DLPFC stimulation. However, none of the stimulation sites showed any significant changes in the dual-task effect. Overall, these results suggest that although tDCS targeting the M1 and DLPFC may immediately enhance motor and cognitive performances, respectively, neither has a significant effect on dual-task interference.

Electrochemical reactor with carbon membrane electrodes for efficient phenol removal via anode and cathode synergism

Current electrochemical membrane reactors (EMRs) focus on half-cell reactions, which limits their efficiency. Herein, an EMR-P with full-cell reactions was constructed using a carbon membrane (CM) as the cathode and a TiO2-loaded CM as the anode. Noteworthy, this proposed innovative design has no ion-exchange membrane and consists of two permeates for anodic electrocatalytic and cathodic electro-Fenton processes. Results showed that the removal rates of phenol and COD by EMR-P were 99.2% and 93.9%, respectively, with energy consumption of 0.43 kWh kg COD–1, which were superior to those of other EMRs. Such superior performance of EMR-P was attributed to the synergism of electro-Fenton and electrocatalytic oxidation, as well as the high adsorption property of CM, which promoted ({1atop}{rm{O}}_{2}) generation and COD removal. Additionally, the cathode made more contribution to the COD removal (59.0%) than the anode (41.0%). Overall, this work provides several insights into the design of EMRs for cleaning industrial wastewater.

Responses

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *