Stress-induced changes in the molecular processes underlying fear memories: implications for PTSD and relevant animal models

Introduction

Our current understanding of stress has evolved from early concepts pioneered by Hippocrates, who postulated that good health relied on maintaining a harmonious balance in physiological functions [1]. Building upon this idea, in the early 20th century, Walter B. Cannon, introduced the term “homeostasis“ to describe the coordinated physiological processes that maintain the organism’s steady states, expanding on Claude Bernard’s concept of the “Milieu intérieur“ from 1870. Although Cannon referenced “stress“ in his work, he never provided a formal definition [2].

Hans Selye extended Cannon’s notions of homeostasis and defined the word “stress” for the first time. He also scientifically measured the physiological changes it induces, although it is now recognized that his postulates do not apply to all species [3]. There is still a lack of consensus on defining stress, although Bruce McEwen’s perspective is widely embraced. “Stress may be defined as a real or interpreted threat to the physiological or psychological integrity of an individual that results in physiological and/or behavioral responses” [4]. His holistic approach also encompasses the positive and negative implications of stress responses [5], rendering it useful for application in preclinical and clinical research contexts.

The primary stress responses found in all vertebrates involve activating two key systems: the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and the sympatho-medullo-adrenal (SAM) axis. Activation of the HPA axis leads to increased levels of adrenocorticotropic hormone and glucocorticoids (such as cortisol in most mammals and corticosterone in rodents like rats and mice) in the bloodstream. The glucocorticoid receptors involved in regulating the HPA axis during stress are the glucocorticoid receptors (GR) and mineralocorticoid receptors (MR) [6]. Activation of the SAM axis results in elevated levels of catecholamines (like noradrenaline and adrenaline) in the bloodstream, and changes in the cardiovascular system and other bodily functions [7]. Fear responses activate these stress systems, leading to adaptative mechanisms such as hormonal and cellular events that help to confront or avoid the threatening stimulus [8].

Michael Davis’s definition of fear is broadly accepted, notably distinguishing it from anxiety, as it is crucial to differentiate between these closely related concepts. “Fear is prompted by imminent and real danger, and galvanizes active defensive responses. In contrast, anxiety is often elicited by less specific and less predictable threats, or by those that are physically or psychologically more distant” [9]. Joseph LeDoux advocates using the term fear only in people because it is intimately associated with the human consciousness. Therefore, he argues that fear should not be used in animals because we do not know how similar or different their consciousness is from human conscience. For experiments in animals, “threat conditioning” should be used instead of fear conditioning [10]. In literature, fear is often considered a more subjective state whereas threat-related behavior is a more descriptive term for observed behavior in model systems. In this review, fear represents both the subjective state and the objective threat-related behavior observed in animals and humans. One of the first reported fear conditioning experiments was performed in 1920, based on Ivan Pavlov’s conditioning principles [11]. In this experiment, an infant named Albert was exposed several times to a rat and a loud noise simultaneously. Following this initial exposure, the rat’s presentation alone caused Albert to cry (fear response) [12]. In 1928, Harrold Jones replicated and expanded upon these findings by incorporating psychogalvanic responses into analogous experiments. Similar to the skin conductance response (SCR), these responses pioneered objective measures of fear [13] leading to the foundation of the first fear conditioning laboratory. Some years later Estes and Skinner created the first procedure of Pavlovian fear-conditioning in rats and described the extinction of the conditioned responses [14]. Fear research conducted in the laboratory proves especially advantageous when studying memory mechanisms. This controlled environment allows significant control over the presentation of stimuli and the recording of psychophysiological, behavioral and neural responses.

Fear conditioning experiments typically involve an initial session (fear acquisition) that pairs an aversive stimulus (usually an electric shock) with a conditioned stimulus, like an acoustic tone or an image. An image is often used for humans, whereas, with animals, it is often an acoustic tone (cued-fear conditioning) or an experimental context (contextual fear conditioning). In a subsequent session (fear extinction), only the conditioned stimulus is presented without the electric shock, leading to the extinction of the fear response. In humans, the fear extinction test is frequently performed right after fear acquisition, but some studies test it 24 h later.

In animals, the fear extinction test is often performed 24 h after fear acquisition. Some studies on fear processing, especially on animals, also focus on recalling the initial conditioning (fear expression) [15]. The consolidation of the fear extinction memory is measured in the fear extinction recall session. See [15] for a methodological review with detailed explanations of different fear procedures in rodents and humans and a summary in Fig. 1. It should be noted that fear generalization is a process by which fear responses extend from a specific threatening stimulus to other similar but potentially harmless stimuli [15].

Fig. 1: A simplified summary of the fear learning paradigms most often used in humans and rodents.
figure 1

A FPS fear-potentiated startle. US unconditioned stimulus, often an electric shock to the hand (annoying but not painful), an airblast directed towards the larynx or an aversive sound, especially used in children. CS conditioned stimulus. The CS+ is the stimulus paired with the US whereas the CS− is not paired with the US. The startle probe is a brief, sudden, loud noise that elicits an automatic response to close the eyes. The startle response is assessed by placing an electrode under the eye to measure action potentials with electromyography of the orbicularis oculi muscle. The pairing of the CS+ and the startle probe elicits an increase in the CS+ response. B SCR skin conductance response. US unconditioned stimulus, often an electric shock to the hand (annoying but not painful). CS conditioned stimulus. The CS+ is the stimulus paired with the US whereas the CS− is not paired with the US. In the non-dominant hand, this SCR measures emotional arousal to rewarding and aversive stimuli. FPS and SCR can be measured within the same experiment but often only FPS or SCR are measured. C US unconditioned stimulus, an electric footshock. A few mild electric footshocks are delivered in an experimental box. Animals are tested for fear extinction and extinction recall in the same experimental box keeping the context as similar as possible. D The cue is often an acoustic tone, an odor, or a light, which are paired with mild electric footshocks. US unconditioned stimulus, an electric footshock. The fear extinction and fear extinction recall are tested changing the conditions of the experimental box used for fear acquisition so that the rodent presents no contextual fear but cued-fear conditioning. For example, between the different fear phases, the color and lighting of the box should be changed. E The conditioned response is often the FPS or SCR in humans and freezing in rodents. CS blocks groups of CSs binned together.

Full size image

An important advantage of fear conditioning experiments is that the neurocircuitry of fear is highly conserved across species, which allows the translation of results from rodent models to human research and the clinic [16]. Additionally, fear research can be combined with a wide array of techniques such as neuroimaging (e.g. functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI) in humans [15] and optogenetics, designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs (DREADDs) and freely moving calcium imaging recordings in rodents [17]. Despite using different methods and paradigms of fear conditioning in humans (fear-potentiated startle) and rodents (cued-fear conditioning) it has been shown that results can help to uncover shared molecular mechanisms of fear across species [18]. Morever, a recent study has found converging evidence of molecular mechanisms of fear in healthy mice and humans, using a combination of fMRI in humans and optogenetics, DREADDs, and freely moving calcium imaging recordings in mice [19]. Finally, to maximize the translatability of results, we must integrate the different methodologies, analyses, and conceptual frameworks used in humans and rodents to further understand fear memory processing [17].

Memory can be defined as the process by which knowledge of the world is encoded, stored, and later retrieved [20]. This conceptualization postulates that memory involves two stages: short-term memory, which lasts minutes or hours, and long-term memory, which can last days, weeks, or years. The main difference is that long-term memory requires the induction of genes and proteins, while short-term memory does not. Importantly, stress is a key modulator of fear memory mechanisms with implications in the clinic of fear-based disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [21] and PTSD-like animal models [17]. The prevalence of PTSD is around 5,6% in trauma exposed individuals but highly variable depending on many factors such as the type of trauma and peritrauma experienced [22, 23]. Examples of trauma can be a car accident, sexual abuse, military combat and personal assault among others. Women and individuals identifying as trans-masculine present higher vulnerability to PTSD compared to other demographics [23, 24], although the causes are not well understood yet.

Fear acquisition, extinction, and extinction recall in humans with PTSD or rodents exposed to traumatic stress compared to healthy individuals can be quite different [17]. The most consistent result of traumatic stress exposure on fear memories has been generally described as impairment of both fear extinction and extinction recall. Mixed results are found regarding traumatic stress exposure and fear acquisition suggesting either similar or enhanced levels compared to healthy individuals. These results may depend on many factors, including the nature of the traumatic stress and sex differences, with females being more vulnerable to enhanced fear acquisition [25,26,27,28,29,30].

Previous literature provides an understanding of the fear mechanisms of healthy rodents and humans (e.g. [31, 32]). Other reviews have extensively covered data concerning neural activity in various brain regions related to PTSD, stress and fear memory (e.g. [33]).

In this review, the main focus is on the molecular mechanisms of fear memory in patients with PTSD, not healthy individuals, and its relevant animal models. These appropriate models are rodents exposed to acute traumatic stress (PTSD-like models) with subsequent exposure to fear conditioning and extinction. See Fig. 2. Chronic traumatic stress in animals is beyond the scope of this review, as it induces significant components of depression-like behavior in addition to impaired fear extinction [33, 34]. Consequently, chronic traumatic stress may serve as a valuable model for studying PTSD comorbid with depression.

Fig. 2: Fear memories in healthy individuals and individuals exposed to traumatic stress.
figure 2

Studies in rodents and humans agree on fear memory after exposure to traumatic stress, leading to impaired fear extinction and fear extinction recall. Figure inspired by Maren and Holmes [33].

Full size image

As most studies test fear conditioning and extinction in healthy animals and rarely incorporate these methods with traumatic stress exposure, the available literature is limited. Moreover, when the sex of the animals is not specified in this review, it must be assumed to be male subjects, as they have been the most used historically in research. Finally, more efforts should be placed into testing both male and female subjects, as data in neuroscience can present striking sex differences [35]. CS blocks groups of CSs binned together.

Sex differences in stress interacting with fear memories

An important characteristic of PTSD is the inability to extinguish fear in safe environments, leading to impaired fear extinction when using FPS [36] or SCR [37]. Additionally, intrusive distressing thoughts in PTSD are associated with impaired fear extinction in FPS [38]. Patients with PTSD also present impaired extinction recall [39], which has been studied in both men and women, mainly with SCR.

When comparing sexes, women with PTSD show similar or enhanced levels of fear acquisition [40, 41] and men show deficits in fear extinction recall [41] measured with SCR. Both men and women with PTSD and healthy individuals generally present a higher response to the CS+ compared to the CS− [41].

Many acute and chronic stress procedures in rodents induce impaired fear extinction, such as restraint, forced swimming, maternal separation, predator exposure, social defeat and isolation, elevated platform exposure, single-prolonged stress (SPS) and stress immobilization on a board (IMO) [33]. For SPS, rodents undergo a 2-h restraint followed directly by a 20-min forced swim. After a brief recovery period, they are exposed to diethyl ether until they reach a state of anesthesia and become unresponsive [33]. In IMO, rodents are placed in a prone position with the limbs attached to metal mounts on boards [42]. IMO is different from the restraint used in SPS and IMO elicits a greater HPA axis response than other types of less severe restraint methods [42]. E.g. restraint in a tube. Moreover, IMO elicits a more prolonged HPA activation than intense and repeated electrical footshocks in rats [43].

Typically, all these different acute and chronic stress procedures are applied some hours or days before the fear procedures, allowing time for the incubation of PTSD-like symptomatology (e.g. [44]). However, most of these methods have only been combined with fear conditioning procedures in male rodents, showing overall impaired fear extinction. Several studies have also investigated female rodents, and there are findings regarding the stress-induced impaired fear extinction phenotype that do not consistently align with those observed in male rodents highlighting significant sex differences. E.g. SPS [33, 45,46,47,48,49]. However, both males and females previously exposed to IMO present equivalent fear acquisition levels and impaired cued-fear extinction [29, 30]. Moreover, IMO has been used to uncover fear molecular mechanisms of stress-related processes in rodents that may be relevant to PTSD in studies involving data in mice and humans [18, 29].

Finally, when studying stress interacting with fear memories, the differences seen between sexes can be attributed to their underlying mechanisms, which involve sex hormones, genetics, epigenetics, and other factors that are beyond the scope of this review.

Hormones influencing stress and fear memory mechanisms

Some studies directly measure hormonal levels in blood or saliva, and others indirectly by monitoring the menstrual (women) or estrous cycle (female rodents). Rebecca Shansky´s work highlights the importance of considering hormonal levels, especially sex hormones, in preclinical and clinical settings to improve the quality of neuroscience research [35]. Here, I will focus on the most studied hormones important in traumatic stress and subsequent fear memory processing: estradiol and cortisol/corticosterone.

We have recently reviewed the role of estradiol and other relevant sex hormones, such as progesterone and testosterone, in fear memories in healthy individuals [31]. Most of the estradiol is produced by the granulosa cells of the ovaries. This process involves the aromatization of androstenedione (produced in the theca follicular cells) to estrone, followed by the conversion of estrone to estradiol by the enzyme 17β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase [50].

Seminal work by Mohammed Milad established that high estradiol levels enhance fear extinction retention in healthy women and female rodents (See the review [31]). This effect warrants further research in women with PTSD especially when considering their menstrual cycle or endogenous estradiol levels [51, 52]. For example, women diagnosed with PTSD presenting low and high estradiol levels in their blood showed similar levels of fear acquisition with FPS [51]. Moreover, women with PTSD and low estradiol levels show impaired fear extinction [53]. Also, women in the midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle (high progesterone and estradiol levels) diagnosed with PTSD show impaired fear extinction retention using SCR [51]. Ethinylestradiol is a synthetic estrogen widely used in oral hormonal contraceptives (HCs) and it appears to enhance fear extinction in women with PTSD. Women exposed to trauma and exhibiting various PTSD symptoms and using HCs exhibited increased acquisition of fear conditioning and enhanced fear extinction compared to women not using HCs and men [54]. Estradiol has been proposed as an adjunct to prolonged exposure therapy involving fear memories in women with PTSD [55]. Finally, the picture of fear memories and estradiol in patients with PTSD is complex, and further studies are needed to link all the different data.

Studies in rodents exposed to traumatic stress before fear procedures evaluating the role of estradiol are minimal. Recent data suggests that the menstrual or estrous cycle at the moment of traumatic stress (IMO) is not associated with impaired fear extinction in mice and PTSD symptoms in women exposed to sexual abuse [29]. These suggest that endogenous sex hormones or the menstrual/estrous cycle during traumatic stress may not be as important as previously suggested for the development of PTSD [56]. However, exogenous sex hormones could be beneficial after traumatic stress to normalize fear memories. For example, female ovariectomized rats showed that systemic injections of β-estradiol after exposure to the SPS reduced contextual freezing suggesting potential therapeutic effects [57]. Interestingly, estradiol plays a complex role in modulating cortisol and corticosterone levels in rodents and humans, with effects dependent on the species, sex, and context of exposure [58,59,60].

In general, cortisol and corticosterone enhance fear memory consolidation in healthy individuals but impair delayed memory retrieval [61, 62]. However, opposite-sex effects of these glucocorticoids on fear memories in healthy humans and rodents have been described and should be further explored. In people, hydrocortisone impaired the contextualization of fear expression, leading to increased fear generalization in FPS data among women, whereas the opposite pattern was observed in men [63]. In mice, systemic corticosterone enhanced cued-fear memory consolidation in males whereas it impaired it in females [64]. It needs to be noted that the HPA axis in rodents and humans although it has many similitudes also presents important differences among species and sexes [65] that must be considered when translating findings across species.

Similar to other hormones like estradiol, cortisol exhibits different effects on fear memories between healthy individuals and patients with PTSD [66]. Interestingly, patients with PTSD often show low baseline cortisol levels [67], and there is extensive research suggesting that giving glucocorticoids in the aftermath of traumatic stress could prevent the development of PTSD [68]. However, the potential therapeutic effect of administering glucocorticoids in patients with PTSD to normalize fear memories is still a complex issue that depends on many variables, including the timing and the dose of the glucocorticoid. For example, a single glucocorticoid administration to patients with PTSD appears to reduce the recall of fear memories but enhances their extinction measured with SCR [66, 69]. Moreover, it has been shown that using the potent GR agonist dexamethasone reduces exaggerated fear responses to the CS+ during FPS in patients with PTSD [70]. Considering altered baseline levels of cortisol and a likely non-linear relationship between corticosteroid levels and memory function, it may be that stress-induced cortisol brings healthy individuals and patients with PTSD to different places on an inverted U. However, this does not necessarily mean that glucocorticoids are doing different things across groups of individuals, but the effects of fluctuating corticosteroid levels must be interpreted in the context of these baselines. It is also possible that altered glucocorticoid levels in individuals with PTSD may not only alter memory strength, but generalization.

Results in animal models align with findings in humans, but unfortunately, these studies have only been performed on male subjects. For example, IMO leads to increased corticosterone levels during cued-fear conditioning and extinction and impairment of fear extinction [71]. Interestingly, inhibiting corticosterone synthesis with metyrapone during fear memory consolidation disrupted extinction retention in rats previously exposed to SPS [72]. Future studies should include individual differences such as the inclusion of sex as a biological variable (SABV) to provide a more inclusive framework.

In conclusion, different hormones such as estradiol and cortisol/corticosterone, are important in modulating stress and fear memory with potential therapeutic implications. A deeper understanding of these hormonal data and of genetic and epigenetic findings on the FKBP prolyl isomerase 5 gene (FKBP5) – FK506 binding protein 51 (FKBP51), pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating peptide (PACAP) – pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide type I receptor (PAC1R), endocannabinoid (eCB) system and the tropomyosin receptor kinase B (TrkB) – brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in stress and pathological fear memories processing may help us to gain a broader perspective.

Genetics and epigenetics in stress and pathological fear memory mechanisms

FKBP5

FKBP5 is a gene that encodes a protein called FKBP51, a member of the immunophilin protein family. FKBP51 is critical in regulating the body’s stress response and GR signaling [73]. Some essential functions of FKBP5 include acting as a co-chaperone with Hsp90 to modulate GR sensitivity [73].

The FKBP5 gene variant rs1360780 increases susceptibility to PTSD after childhood trauma by modifying GR binding to this gene, leading to persistent FKBP5 activation, glucocorticoid resistance, and stress-response hyperarousal [74, 75]. FKBP5 is associated with fear extinction deficits in both patients with PTSD and the PTSD-like model IMO in mice evaluating amygdala Fkbp5 mRNA levels [76]. Concordantly, dexamethasone administered before testing enhanced fear extinction in mice previously exposed to IMO through methylation of the Fkbp5 gene in the amygdala [71]. These epigenetic changes within the Fkbp5 gene occurred 2 h after fear extinction. Another study in rats exposed to SPS showed enhanced fear retrieval and reduced levels of the Fkbp5 gene in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). Interestingly, this increased Fkbp5 expression is observed 2 h after SPS, followed by cued-fear conditioning [77]. A translational study shows that the GR and FKBP51 form a complex whose level is elevated in the blood of patients with PTSD patients and healthy cued fear-conditioned mice 30 min after training, reducing GR activity. Authors developed a peptide that disrupts this complex, normalizing fear memory processing, suggesting the GR-FKBP51 complex as a potential diagnostic biomarker and therapeutic target for PTSD [78]. It would be interesting to test this hypothesis in a PTSD-like model.

Overall, the epigenetic, genetic, and protein data suggest that changes in Fkbp5 are rapidly triggered by traumatic stress impacting fear extinction and can be modulated by drugs with potential therapeutic properties; otherwise, these changes persist over time, as FKBP5 alterations are observed in patients with PTSD.

The PACAP-PAC1R system

The PACAP-PAC1R system is a crucial regulator of the stress response. PACAP is a peptide that binds to three G protein-coupled receptors: PAC1R, Vasoactive intestinal peptide receptor 1 (VPAC1), and Vasoactive intestinal peptide receptor 2 (VPAC2). PAC1R is the high-affinity, PACAP-selective receptor [79].

A genetic variant (rs2267735) in the PAC1R gene (ADCYAP1R1) is linked to PTSD risk and symptoms in women through estradiol regulation of ADCYAP1R1 [80, 81]. Moreover, the PACAP-PAC1R system plays a significant role in PTSD and fear memory evaluated with FPS in the amygdala and hippocampus in women [82]. Another study found that women with the risk genotype of the ADCYAP1R1 rs2267735 polymorphism exhibit impaired fear extinction [29]. Inhibiting medial amygdala (MeA) neurons projecting to the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMHdm) during IMO rescues both PACAP upregulation in VMHdm and the fear extinction deficit in female mice [29]. Specifically, this study suggests that high levels of PACAP in MeA and VMH during traumatic stress led to vulnerability to deficits in fear extinction. In contrast, low levels of PACAP lead to resilience. Also, higher circulating PACAP levels in women with PTSD are associated with increased connectivity between the amygdala and posterior default mode network regions, which is linked to anxious arousal symptoms like exaggerated startle response [83].

In brief, the PACAP-PAC1R system can be a potential target to prevent or treat PTSD-induced impaired fear extinction. Unfortunately, there are no current safe drugs that are well tolerated in humans that specifically target the PACAP-PAC1R system, limiting for now the potential impact in the clinic.

Endocannabinoid system

The eCB system is a complex cell signaling network composed of eCBS (e.g. anandamide (AEA), 1 and 2-arachidonoyl glycerol (1-AG and 2-AG)), enzymes (e.g. fatty acid amide hydrolase (FAAH)), and cannabinoid receptors. The cannabinoid receptor 1 (CB1R) is mainly found in the central nervous system and the cannabinoid receptor 2 (CB2R) is generally present in the peripheral nervous system and immune cells). These are crucial for regulating physiological and cognitive processes and maintaining homeostasis and balance in the body [84]. The well-studied Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) has as a primary receptor the CB1R [85]. FAAH’s primary role is to catalyze the hydrolysis of endogenous amidated lipids, including AEA and 2-AG [86, 87].

The HPA axis and the eCB system present complex interactions potentially relevant for traumatic stress, PTSD, and fear memory alterations [33, 88]. Moreover, the eCB system has been identified as an important modulator of early life stress and adolescent trauma with consequences in adulthood [89]. Specifically, childhood trauma leads to fluctuating levels of eCB ligands and CB1R across different developmental stages, with increased eCB ligands and decreased CB1R in adulthood. This pattern is consistent in healthy humans and animal studies, whereas severe trauma in adulthood generally results in decreased eCB ligands and increased CB1R [89]. There are studies in adult subjects pointing at traumatic stress and associations with PTSD and PTSD-like behavior in rodents. These data suggest increased levels of eCB ligands (1-AG, 2-AG, or AEA) and altered expression of CB1R in brain areas such as the amygdala, mPFC and hippocampus [89, 90].

The eCB system in healthy individuals modulates fear memories and FAAH inhibition promotes fear extinction in rodents [85]. In humans, reduced FAAH activity increases AEA levels, facilitating fear extinction and reducing anxiety and stress, with decreased amygdala reactivity measured with neuroimaging [33, 89]. The eCB system modulation of healthy fear memory processes is bimodal because the role of CB1R depends on the specific cell type and brain region where they are expressed. On GABAergic neurons, activation of CB1R leads to a decrease in active coping strategies in cued-fear conditioning. Conversely, the activation of CB1R on glutamatergic terminals in the central amygdala induces a decrease in passive coping strategies in cued-fear conditioning. Thus, the equilibrium between GABAergic and glutamatergic transmission provides an appropriate emotional reactivity in physiological conditions [85]. Similarly, it has been suggested that low THC doses preferentially at CB1R on glutamatergic neurons, whereas high THC doses also act at GABAergic neurons. CB1R is less abundant in cortical glutamatergic neurons, but its activation produces more pronounced effects than on GABAergic neurons [91]. It would be interesting to test these results in PTSD-like models.

So far, studies in PTSD-like models have indicated that the eCB system may play a significant role in fear memory alterations. For example, the CB1/CB2 receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 (WIN) given 2 min after exposure to SPS, can prevent the stress-induced impairment of contextual fear conditioning through interactions of the GR and eCB receptors in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) and hippocampus [92]. Moreover, systemic administration of WIN 30, given minutes before 6 repeated social defeat exposures in mice effectively blocked the stress-induced enhancement of contextual fear conditioning [93].

Despite the abovementioned promising results in healthy people and PTSD-like models showing that eCB receptors modulate fear, there is no clear evidence that drugs specifically targeting these receptors have a therapeutic effect on enhancing fear extinction or recall in patients with PTSD. However, a recent study shows that patients with PTSD of both sexes who received a low dose of THC before fear extinction, measured with SCR, showed greater activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala compared to the placebo group, and during fear renewal [94]. This work suggests that THC modulates neural indices of fear in patients with PTSD and warrants further investigation for its potential therapeutic effects. The significant sex differences observed in healthy animal models of fear suggest that including SABV in clinical trials exploring the role of eCB receptors in patients with PTSD and their fear extinction could help identify new therapeutic approaches [95].

Thus, the eCB system plays a crucial role in modulating stress-induced changes in the molecular processes underlying fear memories, particularly during early life and adolescence, potentially influencing PTSD. These molecular processes depend highly on the neuronal cell type population and the brain area of expression. Moreover, the significant sex differences observed in the eCB system may be relevant for understanding the effects of PTSD and PTSD-like models on fear memories.

TrkB-BDNF system

BDNF is a neurotrophin that binds to and activates the TrkB receptor, activating signaling pathways that are important for regulating neuronal excitability, synaptic transmission, and plasticity. Activation of TrkB by BDNF initiates signaling cascades such as the ERK1/2 and CREB pathways, which play essential roles in dendritic arborization and activity-dependent transcription. See [96] for a review.

There is substantial evidence demonstrating the involvement of the TrkB-BDNF system in fear memories of healthy individuals, patients with PTSD and PTSD-like models [97]. Experiments in healthy rats show that the BDNF mRNA levels are upregulated in the CA1 region of the hippocampus after contextual fear conditioning [98] and the BLA in cued-fear conditioning [99]. There is growing interest in TrkB receptor signaling for potential therapeutic use with small molecule TrkB agonists and TrkB agonist antibodies for psychiatric and neurological disorders [96, 100] including PTSD. For example, systemic administration of the TrkB agonist 7,8-dihydroxyflavone (7,8-DHF) one hour before behavior rescues IMO-induced impaired cued-fear extinction in mice [30]. There are no clinical trials dosing TrkB agonists or TrkB agonist antibodies for psychiatric disorders, so our understanding is limited in this regard. However, antidepressants have recently been shown to bind the TrkB’s transmembrane domain dimer [101]. Therefore, the direct binding of antidepressants to TrkB receptors presents a promising opportunity to model alterations in fear memory that characterize PTSD and PTSD-like conditions, given that antidepressants can enhance fear extinction in mice [102].

In humans, the BDNF Val66Met single nucleotide polymorphism is a common and functional variant that affects the activity-dependent release of BDNF [103]. The BDNF Val66Met genotype is linked to increased PTSD risk and impaired fear extinction using SCR; the Val66Met allele correlates with greater PTSD severity and poorer fear extinction compared to the Val–Val allele, which shows no significant relationship between fear extinction and PTSD [104]. Interestingly, healthy women Met-carriers of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism present significantly weaker CS discrimination in fear extinction than non-carriers evaluated with FPS [105]. These data suggest that it is important to further explore the TrkB-BDNF system in PTSD and fear extinction considering SABV.

Further studies on the TrkB-BDNF system with either antidepressants, TrkB agonists or TrkB agonist antibodies hold promise for potentially treating PTSD fear memory alterations.

Limitations

It is important to acknowledge that this review focuses on a small number of molecular pathways implicated in trauma, PTSD, and fear. Others, including differential glutamatergic/NMDA regulation, monoamine and adrenergic modulation, cholinergic modulation, and other neuropeptides such as tachykinins, ghrelin, vasopressin, and angiotensin have not been discussed herein.

Moreover, when discussing some genetic evidence for particular pathways of this review, it is important to note that these genes have not yet survived large-scale genome-wide association studies of PTSD. For example, the most extensive genome-wide association study (GWAS) of more than 1 million subjects for PTSD [106]. While there may be many reasons why common genetic polymorphisms have been identified in smaller studies and do not survive very large GWAS, it is important to clarify that they have not yet been shown to be significant in these “gold standard” genetic studies.

Conclusions

The study of stress-induced molecular mechanisms of fear memory processes with implications for PTSD and relevant animal models is advancing towards more personalized approaches, including SABV and expanding from early life periods to adulthood. However, there are still critical gaps in the literature, such as the lack of more effective pharmacological compounds that humans can use to modulate pathological fear memories. Another gap in our current understanding is that the vast majority of animal and human fear conditioning and extinction studies in PTSD have been conducted in male adults, with little research on females, adolescents and children.

Future studies should aim to integrate efforts across laboratories to maximize the use of resources and expertise. A recent groundbreaking study on stress-related disorders exemplifies how the field should address its challenges by employing an integrative systems approach that combines multi-region and multi-omic analyses with a large sample of individuals with PTSD or depression, and controls [107]. The results of such a study show differences and similitudes between PTSD and depression, uncovering potential risk and vulnerability biomarkers and therapeutic targets. Although it did not include fear memory measures, combining this data with future FPS and SCR experiments and relevant PTSD models could be crucial for understanding stress-induced changes in the molecular processes underlying fear memories.

Perhaps we should reconsider the idea that testing healthy rodents, which have not been previously exposed to traumatic stress, in fear procedures can be classified as a PTSD-like model, as it may be more comparable to fear responses observed in healthy humans.

Related Articles

Compartmentalized dendritic plasticity in the mouse retrosplenial cortex links contextual memories formed close in time

Events occurring close in time are often linked in memory, and recent studies suggest that such memories are encoded by overlapping neuronal ensembles. However, the role of dendritic plasticity mechanisms in linking memories is unknown. Here we show that memory linking is dependent not only on neuronal ensemble overlap in the mouse retrosplenial cortex, but also on branch-specific dendritic allocation mechanisms. The same dendritic segments are preferentially activated by two linked (but not independent) contextual memories, and spine clusters added after each of two linked (but not independent) contextual memories are allocated to the same dendritic segments. Importantly, we show that the reactivation of dendrites activated during the first context exploration is sufficient to link two contextual memories. Our results demonstrate a critical role for localized dendritic plasticity in memory integration and reveal rules governing how linked and independent memories are allocated to dendritic compartments.

A unified acoustic-to-speech-to-language embedding space captures the neural basis of natural language processing in everyday conversations

This study introduces a unified computational framework connecting acoustic, speech and word-level linguistic structures to study the neural basis of everyday conversations in the human brain. We used electrocorticography to record neural signals across 100 h of speech production and comprehension as participants engaged in open-ended real-life conversations. We extracted low-level acoustic, mid-level speech and contextual word embeddings from a multimodal speech-to-text model (Whisper). We developed encoding models that linearly map these embeddings onto brain activity during speech production and comprehension. Remarkably, this model accurately predicts neural activity at each level of the language processing hierarchy across hours of new conversations not used in training the model. The internal processing hierarchy in the model is aligned with the cortical hierarchy for speech and language processing, where sensory and motor regions better align with the model’s speech embeddings, and higher-level language areas better align with the model’s language embeddings. The Whisper model captures the temporal sequence of language-to-speech encoding before word articulation (speech production) and speech-to-language encoding post articulation (speech comprehension). The embeddings learned by this model outperform symbolic models in capturing neural activity supporting natural speech and language. These findings support a paradigm shift towards unified computational models that capture the entire processing hierarchy for speech comprehension and production in real-world conversations.

Constructing future behavior in the hippocampal formation through composition and replay

The hippocampus is critical for memory, imagination and constructive reasoning. Recent models have suggested that its neuronal responses can be well explained by state spaces that model the transitions between experiences. Here we use simulations and hippocampal recordings to reconcile these views. We show that if state spaces are constructed compositionally from existing building blocks, or primitives, hippocampal responses can be interpreted as compositional memories, binding these primitives together. Critically, this enables agents to behave optimally in new environments with no new learning, inferring behavior directly from the composition. We predict a role for hippocampal replay in building and consolidating these compositional memories. We test these predictions in two datasets by showing that replay events from newly discovered landmarks induce and strengthen new remote firing fields. When the landmark is moved, replay builds a new firing field at the same vector to the new location. Together, these findings provide a framework for reasoning about compositional memories and demonstrate that such memories are formed in hippocampal replay.

Pathogen stress heightens sensorimotor dimensions in the human collective semantic space

Infectious diseases have been major causes of death throughout human history and are assumed to broadly affect human psychology. However, whether and how conceptual processing, an internal world model central to various cognitive processes, adapts to such salient stress variables remains largely unknown. To address this, we conducted three studies examining the relationship between pathogen severity and semantic space, probed through the main neurocognitive semantic dimensions revealed by large-scale text analyses: one cross-cultural study (across 43 countries) and two historical studies (over the past 100 years). Across all three studies, we observed that increasing pathogen severity was associated with an enhancement of the sensory-motor dimension in the collective semantic space. These patterns remained robust after controlling for the effects of sociocultural variables, including economic wealth and societal norms of tightness. These results highlight the universal dynamic mechanisms of collective semantics, such that pathogen stress potentially drives sensorially oriented semantic processing.

Responses

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *